1  Experiments and experimental design

There are two fundamental ways to obtain information in research: by observation or by experimentation. In an observational study the observer watches and records information about the subject of interest. In an experiment, the experimenter actively manipulates variables hypothesized to affect the response (insert small example). Although both are important ways of understanding the world around us, only through experiments can we infer causality.

That is, by designing and conducting an experiment properly, if we observe a result such as a change in variable A leads to a change in our response (say variable B), we can confidently conclude that A caused this change in B. If we were to merely study variable B and observe that as variable A changes, B also changes without conducting an experiment, then we can only say that variable A and B are associated. We could not easily conclude that any change in B is due to A. It could be some other factor that is correlated with A or it could be that B caused the change in A! The key is that a well-designed experiment controls and holds constant (as best we can) all other factors that might affect the response, so we can be sure the result is caused by the variable we manipulated.

Imagine a company wants to determine whether their voluntary employee training program (the explanatory variable) increases productivity (the response). They decide to track the productivity of employees who chose to complete the training and those who did not. They note that, on average, trained employees are more productive. Can we confidently conclude that the training program caused increased productivity?

This is an observational study since no variable was actively manipulated, they merely observed and recorded the productivity of two groups of employees. So, we cannot conclude that completing the training program increases productivity - we cannot infer causality. It could be due to many other factors, either observed or unobserved, such as maybe employees who choose to do the training program are inherently more motivated and thus productive. Can you think of any other factors?

If they actively manipulate the explanatory variable, training program, by randomly assigning employees to complete the training program or not and control other factors by ensuring the employees are as similar as possible accross the groups (i.e. conducted an experiment). Any differences in productivity between the two groups could then be ascribed to the training program. If they happen to find that the employees who were assigned the training program are more productive, they can confidently say that the program caused increased productivity (and perhaps make it compulsory for all employees!).

Experimental studies are extremely important in research and in practice. They are almost the only way in which one can control all factors to such an extent as to eliminate any other possible explanation for a change in a response other than the variable actively manipulated. In this course, we only consider experimental studies and those which aim to compare the effects of a number of treatments (comparative experiments).

Here are some other reasons for conducting experiments:

  1. They are easy to analyse. A well designed experiment results in independent estimates of treatment effects which allow us to easily interpret the effects.

  2. Experiments are frequently used to find optimal levels of variables which will maximise (or minimise) the response. Such experiments can save enormous amounts of time and money. Imagine trying to find the optimal settings for producing electricity from coal without proper experimentation. Such a trial and error process would be extremely costly, wasteful and time consuming. In a similar vein, what if the fictional company in our previous example decided to invest a bunch of money in fine-tuning their training program based solely on the results of an observational study. In reality though, it turns out that adjusting their hiring process to identify more keen candidates would have been much more efficient and inexpensive.

  3. In an experiment we can choose exactly those settings or treatment levels we are interested in, e.g. we can investigate the effect of different shift lengths (6, 8 or 9 hours) on employee productivity or test specific price points (R100, R150, R200) to determine which price maximizes sales or revenue. We can actively manipulate the variable(s) to the levels we are interested in.

Experimental studies and their design are fundamental to science, allowing us to further knowledge and test theories. So lets define them more rigorously. We’ll start by introducing some terminology.

Establishing causality through observation is possible, but a bit more difficult.

Experiments are the most reliable way to establish causation because they involve direct manipulation of variables and control for other factors that might influence the outcome. By ensuring that differences in results are due to the specific factor being studied, experiments help avoid misleading conclusions caused by external influences or chance associations.

However, in some cases, causation can still be inferred from observational studies, especially when there is a well-understood relationship between cause and effect, consistent patterns across different settings, and no plausible alternative explanations. For example, the link between smoking and lung cancer was established through observational data, where researchers accounted for other possible influences and found strong, consistent evidence that smoking increases cancer risk. While experiments are preferred, careful analysis and logical reasoning can sometimes provide enough evidence for causal claims without direct intervention.

Key points

  1. Two ways of doing research: observation and expermentation.
  2. Experimentation is the path to causality.
  3. Experiments actively manipulate variables to isolate their effects on a response while controlling everything else.
  4. We consider comparative experiments where the aim is to compare treatments.